亚洲免费av电影一区二区三区,日韩爱爱视频,51精品视频一区二区三区,91视频爱爱,日韩欧美在线播放视频,中文字幕少妇AV,亚洲电影中文字幕,久久久久亚洲av成人网址,久久综合视频网站,国产在线不卡免费播放

        ?

        Human memory across nations and the potential for optimizing learning

        2012-04-29 12:48:02石妮
        都市家教·上半月 2012年6期
        關(guān)鍵詞:教學(xué)研究語(yǔ)文

        You are studying a foreign language and you must learn 30 words from yesterdays class. How can you achieve this with the fewest number of tries and retain the words for the long run?

        Perhaps you may learn the words in three sessions of 10 minutes each if you group them under pertinent categories, visualize their characteristics, invent mnemonics, link them to related concepts. But to memorize them in a hodgepodge order you may need 5 20-minute sessions – and later forget more. And overall, you may recall more words a year later if you rehearse after one month than if you rehearse them after one week.

        Will the same learning methods work for the languages of the Pygmies as well as for French? the inhabitants of Nauru or the Canadian First Nations? High-income countries as well as developing countries?

        They probably will. All humans belong to one species, so despite individual and cultural differences, we process information in the same general ways. Our memory is like a huge bottle with a very narrow neck. We can retain infinite amounts of information, but at any given moment we can process very little of it. This feature influences our thinking and the way our languages are made. So, languages and scripts appear to be very different across the earth, but in fact they conform to memory specifications.

        The commonalities of human cognition can help make the education of the poor in low-income countries more efficient. Instructional methods that dovetail the ways people are set up to retain information may educate more students and save money and time.

        The learning efficiency concept has received little attention thus far, partly because high-income countries have little need for it in the early grades. (Studies like Pashler et al. 2007 aim at higher knowledge levels.) In higher-income countries, curricula and timetables have some redundancy to allow for repeated learning opportunities, so even lower-performing children acquire basic skills. But in poor countries instructional time is often wasted, and students have little opportunity to catch up on something they miss. Trained teachers are scarce, and schools may be already used in three shifts. It becomes important therefore, to use methods that help students encode and retain as much as possible from every instructional hour.

        Such methods must be identified, promoted, and put to use. Some hypotheses exist, but methods must be specified for efficiency. For example, how can poorer students be taught to recognize all letters in, say 15 days rather than 30? Systematic instruction of letter analogies probably speeds up retention, but by how much, compared to alternatives? How often should activities change to avoid attention span loss? It is known that certain movements consolidate memory, but which ones will facilitate retention of specific items? These issues seem immaterial where students have abundant opportunities to learn. But in countries where student lives and millions of dollars are at stake, such details may help move some students from early dropout to basic skills acquisition. To retain new items, we must know the prerequisites. Therefore to progress meaningfully to higher grades, efficiency in acquiring basic skills is a crucial topic.

        Interestingly, learning efficiency is a concept that intersects with educational economics. Economists study the internal efficiency of school systems as portrayed by student flows: dropout, repetition, attainment rates. The unit of measurement is the student, but the underlying construct is learning efficiency. Perhaps, retention of information per unit of instructional time could be quantified. This would be more useful for the early grades when information is fairly concrete and essential. The uniformity of human cognition would facilitate generalizations.

        But in an age where individual differences and cultural sensitivities are celebrated, emphasis on underlying uniformities may arouse philosophical questions. Individual attention to students would be highly desirable, but the schools of the poor give limited opportunities for this. What is the role of our cognitive commonalities vis a vis our cultural differences? Readers views would be greatly appreciated.

        作者簡(jiǎn)介:

        石妮(1978-),女,1978年生,廣西桂林人,廣西桂林市樂(lè)群小學(xué)教導(dǎo)處主任,學(xué)歷:本科,主要從事小學(xué)語(yǔ)文教學(xué)研究。

        猜你喜歡
        教學(xué)研究語(yǔ)文
        《快樂(lè)語(yǔ)文》2022年全新改版!
        《快樂(lè)語(yǔ)文》2022年全新改版!
        《快樂(lè)語(yǔ)文》2022年全新改版!
        高中數(shù)學(xué)復(fù)習(xí)課教學(xué)研究
        高中數(shù)學(xué)教學(xué)研究
        交替?zhèn)髯g中聽(tīng)記平衡教學(xué)研究
        中職高考與教學(xué)研究
        語(yǔ)文課堂有效教學(xué)研究
        《protel DXP 2004》的教學(xué)研究
        河南科技(2014年5期)2014-02-27 14:08:56
        亚洲av综合国产av日韩| 亚洲电影久久久久久久9999| 久久人妻av不卡中文字幕| 日韩在线不卡一区三区av| 国内精品视频一区二区三区八戒| 波多野结衣av手机在线观看| 亚洲女同精品一区二区久久| 日本大片在线一区二区三区 | 国产综合精品久久久久成人| 亚洲中文字幕一区av| 亚洲爆乳无码精品aaa片蜜桃| 老外和中国女人毛片免费视频| 日本中文字幕不卡在线一区二区| 中文字幕亚洲精品高清| 国内精品免费一区二区三区| 亚洲综合欧美在线一区在线播放 | 九一精品少妇一区二区三区| 国内精品久久久久影院优| 久久99精品国产99久久6男男| 美女视频很黄很a免费国产| 99久久精品人妻一区| 久久久久国产精品| 免费无码成人av在线播| 欧美片欧美日韩国产综合片| 亚洲日本一区二区三区四区| 欧美老熟妇喷水| 自拍偷拍亚洲一区| 亚洲一区二区三区新视频| 级毛片内射视频| 国产精品美女一区二区三区 | 久久精品国产亚洲av忘忧草18| 亚洲色四在线视频观看| 亚洲综合中文字幕日韩| 欧美牲交a欧美牲交aⅴ免费真| 丰满岳乱妇在线观看中字无码| 国产乱老熟视频乱老熟女1| 国产av自拍视频在线观看| 一区二区三区国产亚洲网站| 国产性一交一乱一伦一色一情 | 日韩激情无码免费毛片| 国产精品亚洲片夜色在线|