亚洲免费av电影一区二区三区,日韩爱爱视频,51精品视频一区二区三区,91视频爱爱,日韩欧美在线播放视频,中文字幕少妇AV,亚洲电影中文字幕,久久久久亚洲av成人网址,久久综合视频网站,国产在线不卡免费播放

        ?

        Gender and Language Usages From a Socio—cultural Perspective

        2018-05-29 09:59:34ShanChen
        校園英語·中旬 2018年3期

        Shan Chen

        【Abstract】Females and males use language in a very different way regarding their pronunciations, vocabulary choices, sentence structures and pragmatic meanings. This paper is to list the major differences in gender-related language usages, and then the primary socio-cultural reasons will be presented to analyze these differences.

        【Key words】language usages; females; males; power; social expectations

        【作者簡介】Shan Chen, Changde Vocational Technical College.

        Males and females are obligatory to make choices in their daily life. These choices may be manifest in the words they opt for to express themselves, the tones and pitches they articulate, the sentence structures they construct and the meanings they intend to convey and actually convey. This paper aims to list some of the divergences among the linguistic items adopted by males and females, and then the primarily related socio-cultural reasons will be explored.

        1. Differences in Language Usages

        One can quickly be picked up among the crowds, not only by their appearances, but also by their styles especially their language styles. When it comes to telling the differences between women and men, this also applies. Various language usages, including pronunciations, vocabulary, sentences and pragmatics, can help to delimit the boundary existing between males and females.

        1.1 Pronunciations

        Initially Lavob has shown a stratification of phonological variables according to gender, age, socioeconomic status, and situational context. Statistically, in an early study of children, Fischer (1958) found that boys used a higher frequency of final -in as opposed to -ing than girls. In the 24 children he studied, only 17% of the girls used more -in than -ing, in contrast to 58% of the boys. In Singapore English, Hiang & Gupta (1992) found that postvocalic r occurred more frequently in females than in males (cited in Major, 2004).

        Qualitatively, take Japanese as an illustration, watashi (I) is often pronounced as atashi by female, which can show a smaller mouth shape appreciated by their society. Moreover, interjections including the pronouncing of /ɑ/ are used more frequently by male, while /i / by female. It seems that women and men are inclined to adopt separate pronunciations to indicate their divergence.

        1.2 Vocabulary

        Males and females are found to use disparate ways to memorize vocabulary, from which yielded different lexical production (e.g. Murphy, 2011), just as Bouchard, etc. (2009) revealed that girls and boys follow different developmental paths of language acquisition, namely, between 8 and 30 months, girls understand more sentences and produce more words than boys, but boys catch up with girls and then seem to surpass them during the 30th month.

        Growing up, females use more “empty” adjectives, such as adorable, charming, divine and nice. They also use more intensifiers than men, such as “I feel so happy” and “The movie is very moving”. Besides, women use hedges more than men do, just like “Its kinda nice” (Freeman & McElhinny, 2001).

        1.3 Sentences

        It seems that women always use the correct and formal grammar in their sentence productions, which isnt always the case for men.

        Females use more tag questions than males. They frequently express such sentences as “The weather is really nice today, isnt it?” In other situations, women utilize question intonation in statements to express uncertainty like “My name is Tammy?” (Freeman & McElhinny, 2001). Compared with women, men dispose their utterances to a casual and simple direction, which can be effortlessly understood in that men neednt others to echo them, and they are confident to be the predominant positions to assert what they want to say.

        Very often, its stereotyped that female talk or gossip too much, and therefore women can generate more sentences than men in the conversations. However, this bias excludes the very fact that men actually talk more than women in meetings, television talk shows, and classrooms. Hence, it makes sense to hold that the amount of sentences is distributed differently in womens and mens utterances, with more of womens in some informal context, while more of mens in some relatively formal context.

        1.4 Pragmatics

        Unlike women who tend to hide or imply their actual meanings in their speeches, men are often straight forward, directly meaning what they want others to believe. Therefore, it is traditionally assumed that womens speech is more polite than mens. Major (2004) reported in his study that male speakers showed a higher percentage of casual or less prestigious speech forms than females, and that the male and female speakers used more casual forms in the less formal style, and in both styles males used more casual forms than females.

        Even using the same words or phrases, underlying emotions of males and females, may vary from each other. Holmes finds that men use you know slightly more often to express linguistic imprecision, which is contrary to the fact that women use you know more frequently to express emphasis or attribute knowledge to another speaker, and men and women using you know at approximately the same rates to express appeals and conjoint knowledge (cited in Freeman & McRlhinny, 2001).

        2. Socio-cultural Reasons for Those Differences

        This part is to select two major social factors pertained to gender-linked differences in language usage—power and social expectations to explain why women and men resort to different language styles.

        2.1 Power or Social Identities

        Conventionally, men are responsible for outside work to support their family, whereas women are obligatory to work inside to maintain the sound running of the whole family. Different social assignments implicitly specify womens and mens occupation in their daily life. Power or social identities influences both males and females when it comes to decision-making, of course, language usage being one of the many different choices. Its widely accepted that men use hard language to show their predominant position in the world, while women adopt soft language to manifest their subordinate position to men.

        The dual-culture model proposed by Tannen holds that many men, as individuals in a hierarchical social order, approach the world where they try to achieve and maintain the upper hand if they can, and protect themselves from others attempts to put them down and push them around. Life, then, is a contest, a struggle to preserve independence and avoid failure. However, women approach the world as individuals in a network of connections. They try to seek and give confirmation and support, and to reach consensus. They try to protect themselves from others attempts to push them away. Life, then, is a community, a struggle to preserve intimacy and avoid isolation. Male and female display their unique ways to eye the social world (cited in Hudson, 2000). Correspondingly, language is one of their tools to convey their different social values and thought patterns, on the ground that men are always trying to grasp the opportunities to show off their power, while women always manage to veil their inferior positions, so as to confirm to their social identities.

        2.2 Social Expectations

        Unequal social power between males and females, naturally, leads to different social valuations and requirements exerted by the society them.

        Hammer, etc. (2011) conducted a longitudinal investigation and concluded that the childrens gender affects mothers language usage to their children because mothers are amenable to different acculturation styles when interacting with their daughters and sons. This is tantamount to acknowledging that different social expectations can instill different inputs to males and females in their childhood, resulting in their various language comprehension and language production.

        Therefore, just as Lakoff argues, a female faces a double bind. If she does not learn to behavior like a lady, she will be criticized, ostracized, or scolded. If, on the other hand, she does learn to behavior like a lady, she will be systematically denied access to power on the ground that she is not capable of holding it. Trapped by this dilemma, to some degree, the more education a female receive, the more etiquettes she will obey and the more familiar she will become to the social rules underpinned for the sake of traditional education and other social members expectations (Freeman & McRlhinny, 2001 ). It is implied, therefore, that education can narrow the biased social expectations attached to females and males.

        The role of gender cannot be overemphasized and excluded out of context, variation, intra-group difference and inter-group overlapping. There exist complicated relations between language and gender. Multiple perspectives should be taken to approach the issue at the textual level, since most gendered features are represented in texts. Research cannot separate the parameter of gender from other social and contextual factors.

        References:

        [1]Bouchard,C.,Trudeau,N.,Sutton,A.,Boudreault,M.&Deneault;,J.2009. Gender differences in language development in French Canadian children between 8 and 30 months of age. Applied Pycholinguistics, 30:685–707.

        [2]Freeman,R.&McElhinny;,B.2001.Language and Gender.In Sandra,L.M.& Nornberger,N.H.Sociolinguistics and Language Teaching.Cambridge:Cambridge University Press/ FLTRP.

        [3]Hammer,C.S.,Lawrence,F(xiàn).,Rodriguez,B.,Davison,M.D.& Miccio,A.W.2011.Changes in language usage of Puerto Rican mothers and their children:Do gender and timing of exposure to English matter? Applied Psycholinguistics,32:275–297.

        [4]Hudson,R.A.,2000.Sociolinguistics .Foreign Language Teaching and Research Press/ Cambridge University Press.

        [5]Major,R.C.2004.Gender and stylistic variation in second language phonology.Language Variation and Change,16:169–188.

        久久精品国产亚洲av久按摩 | 中文字幕一区二区中出后入| 内射欧美老妇wbb| 豆国产95在线 | 亚洲| 无码AV大香线蕉伊人久久| 国产在线观看女主播户外| 蜜臀av在线观看| 另类内射国产在线| 亚洲色成人WWW永久在线观看| 久久夜色精品国产亚洲av老牛 | 中文无码成人免费视频在线观看| 精品久久久久久中文字幕| 免费 无码 国产精品| 中文字幕乱码一区在线观看| 色综合久久无码五十路人妻| 国产精品久久婷婷六月丁香| 久久国产精品免费一区六九堂| 91精品国产九色综合久久香蕉| 无码爆乳护士让我爽| 欧美婷婷六月丁香综合色| 欧美片欧美日韩国产综合片| 青青草成人免费在线观看视频 | 久久国产精久久精产国| 国产成人精品cao在线| 玖玖资源站亚洲最大的网站| 国产伦精品免编号公布| 精品国产看高清国产毛片| 国产目拍亚洲精品二区| 国产激情久久久久影院小草| 亚洲av永久无码精品国产精品 | 加勒比黑人在线| 亚洲av调教捆绑一区二区三区| 久久精品中文闷骚内射| 丝袜美腿一区二区国产| 成人爽a毛片在线视频| 亚洲都市校园激情另类| 中文字幕一区二区三区综合网| 婷婷综合另类小说色区| 欧美性群另类交| 亚洲国产综合性感三级自拍| 久久国产精品一区二区三区|