亚洲免费av电影一区二区三区,日韩爱爱视频,51精品视频一区二区三区,91视频爱爱,日韩欧美在线播放视频,中文字幕少妇AV,亚洲电影中文字幕,久久久久亚洲av成人网址,久久综合视频网站,国产在线不卡免费播放

        ?

        Gender and Language Usages From a Socio—cultural Perspective

        2018-05-29 09:59:34ShanChen
        校園英語(yǔ)·中旬 2018年3期
        關(guān)鍵詞:簡(jiǎn)介

        Shan Chen

        【Abstract】Females and males use language in a very different way regarding their pronunciations, vocabulary choices, sentence structures and pragmatic meanings. This paper is to list the major differences in gender-related language usages, and then the primary socio-cultural reasons will be presented to analyze these differences.

        【Key words】language usages; females; males; power; social expectations

        【作者簡(jiǎn)介】Shan Chen, Changde Vocational Technical College.

        Males and females are obligatory to make choices in their daily life. These choices may be manifest in the words they opt for to express themselves, the tones and pitches they articulate, the sentence structures they construct and the meanings they intend to convey and actually convey. This paper aims to list some of the divergences among the linguistic items adopted by males and females, and then the primarily related socio-cultural reasons will be explored.

        1. Differences in Language Usages

        One can quickly be picked up among the crowds, not only by their appearances, but also by their styles especially their language styles. When it comes to telling the differences between women and men, this also applies. Various language usages, including pronunciations, vocabulary, sentences and pragmatics, can help to delimit the boundary existing between males and females.

        1.1 Pronunciations

        Initially Lavob has shown a stratification of phonological variables according to gender, age, socioeconomic status, and situational context. Statistically, in an early study of children, Fischer (1958) found that boys used a higher frequency of final -in as opposed to -ing than girls. In the 24 children he studied, only 17% of the girls used more -in than -ing, in contrast to 58% of the boys. In Singapore English, Hiang & Gupta (1992) found that postvocalic r occurred more frequently in females than in males (cited in Major, 2004).

        Qualitatively, take Japanese as an illustration, watashi (I) is often pronounced as atashi by female, which can show a smaller mouth shape appreciated by their society. Moreover, interjections including the pronouncing of /ɑ/ are used more frequently by male, while /i / by female. It seems that women and men are inclined to adopt separate pronunciations to indicate their divergence.

        1.2 Vocabulary

        Males and females are found to use disparate ways to memorize vocabulary, from which yielded different lexical production (e.g. Murphy, 2011), just as Bouchard, etc. (2009) revealed that girls and boys follow different developmental paths of language acquisition, namely, between 8 and 30 months, girls understand more sentences and produce more words than boys, but boys catch up with girls and then seem to surpass them during the 30th month.

        Growing up, females use more “empty” adjectives, such as adorable, charming, divine and nice. They also use more intensifiers than men, such as “I feel so happy” and “The movie is very moving”. Besides, women use hedges more than men do, just like “Its kinda nice” (Freeman & McElhinny, 2001).

        1.3 Sentences

        It seems that women always use the correct and formal grammar in their sentence productions, which isnt always the case for men.

        Females use more tag questions than males. They frequently express such sentences as “The weather is really nice today, isnt it?” In other situations, women utilize question intonation in statements to express uncertainty like “My name is Tammy?” (Freeman & McElhinny, 2001). Compared with women, men dispose their utterances to a casual and simple direction, which can be effortlessly understood in that men neednt others to echo them, and they are confident to be the predominant positions to assert what they want to say.

        Very often, its stereotyped that female talk or gossip too much, and therefore women can generate more sentences than men in the conversations. However, this bias excludes the very fact that men actually talk more than women in meetings, television talk shows, and classrooms. Hence, it makes sense to hold that the amount of sentences is distributed differently in womens and mens utterances, with more of womens in some informal context, while more of mens in some relatively formal context.

        1.4 Pragmatics

        Unlike women who tend to hide or imply their actual meanings in their speeches, men are often straight forward, directly meaning what they want others to believe. Therefore, it is traditionally assumed that womens speech is more polite than mens. Major (2004) reported in his study that male speakers showed a higher percentage of casual or less prestigious speech forms than females, and that the male and female speakers used more casual forms in the less formal style, and in both styles males used more casual forms than females.

        Even using the same words or phrases, underlying emotions of males and females, may vary from each other. Holmes finds that men use you know slightly more often to express linguistic imprecision, which is contrary to the fact that women use you know more frequently to express emphasis or attribute knowledge to another speaker, and men and women using you know at approximately the same rates to express appeals and conjoint knowledge (cited in Freeman & McRlhinny, 2001).

        2. Socio-cultural Reasons for Those Differences

        This part is to select two major social factors pertained to gender-linked differences in language usage—power and social expectations to explain why women and men resort to different language styles.

        2.1 Power or Social Identities

        Conventionally, men are responsible for outside work to support their family, whereas women are obligatory to work inside to maintain the sound running of the whole family. Different social assignments implicitly specify womens and mens occupation in their daily life. Power or social identities influences both males and females when it comes to decision-making, of course, language usage being one of the many different choices. Its widely accepted that men use hard language to show their predominant position in the world, while women adopt soft language to manifest their subordinate position to men.

        The dual-culture model proposed by Tannen holds that many men, as individuals in a hierarchical social order, approach the world where they try to achieve and maintain the upper hand if they can, and protect themselves from others attempts to put them down and push them around. Life, then, is a contest, a struggle to preserve independence and avoid failure. However, women approach the world as individuals in a network of connections. They try to seek and give confirmation and support, and to reach consensus. They try to protect themselves from others attempts to push them away. Life, then, is a community, a struggle to preserve intimacy and avoid isolation. Male and female display their unique ways to eye the social world (cited in Hudson, 2000). Correspondingly, language is one of their tools to convey their different social values and thought patterns, on the ground that men are always trying to grasp the opportunities to show off their power, while women always manage to veil their inferior positions, so as to confirm to their social identities.

        2.2 Social Expectations

        Unequal social power between males and females, naturally, leads to different social valuations and requirements exerted by the society them.

        Hammer, etc. (2011) conducted a longitudinal investigation and concluded that the childrens gender affects mothers language usage to their children because mothers are amenable to different acculturation styles when interacting with their daughters and sons. This is tantamount to acknowledging that different social expectations can instill different inputs to males and females in their childhood, resulting in their various language comprehension and language production.

        Therefore, just as Lakoff argues, a female faces a double bind. If she does not learn to behavior like a lady, she will be criticized, ostracized, or scolded. If, on the other hand, she does learn to behavior like a lady, she will be systematically denied access to power on the ground that she is not capable of holding it. Trapped by this dilemma, to some degree, the more education a female receive, the more etiquettes she will obey and the more familiar she will become to the social rules underpinned for the sake of traditional education and other social members expectations (Freeman & McRlhinny, 2001 ). It is implied, therefore, that education can narrow the biased social expectations attached to females and males.

        The role of gender cannot be overemphasized and excluded out of context, variation, intra-group difference and inter-group overlapping. There exist complicated relations between language and gender. Multiple perspectives should be taken to approach the issue at the textual level, since most gendered features are represented in texts. Research cannot separate the parameter of gender from other social and contextual factors.

        References:

        [1]Bouchard,C.,Trudeau,N.,Sutton,A.,Boudreault,M.&Deneault;,J.2009. Gender differences in language development in French Canadian children between 8 and 30 months of age. Applied Pycholinguistics, 30:685–707.

        [2]Freeman,R.&McElhinny;,B.2001.Language and Gender.In Sandra,L.M.& Nornberger,N.H.Sociolinguistics and Language Teaching.Cambridge:Cambridge University Press/ FLTRP.

        [3]Hammer,C.S.,Lawrence,F(xiàn).,Rodriguez,B.,Davison,M.D.& Miccio,A.W.2011.Changes in language usage of Puerto Rican mothers and their children:Do gender and timing of exposure to English matter? Applied Psycholinguistics,32:275–297.

        [4]Hudson,R.A.,2000.Sociolinguistics .Foreign Language Teaching and Research Press/ Cambridge University Press.

        [5]Major,R.C.2004.Gender and stylistic variation in second language phonology.Language Variation and Change,16:169–188.

        猜你喜歡
        簡(jiǎn)介
        The Training Mode on Engineering Education
        No abuse of Audio—visual Media in Engineering surveying Teaching
        Short—term Memory Training in Business English Interpreting
        HowtoApplyEquityandInclusioninLanguageTeaching
        The Application of Authentic Listening Materials in EFL Classes
        Life of Foreign Students In China
        ChapteronTheRefugeeCrisis:AChancefortheEU
        Research on Guidance Mechanism of Public Opinion in Colleges and Universities in Micro Era
        Book review on “Educating Elites”
        Values of Medical Videos in Medical English Instruction
        日日摸日日碰夜夜爽无码| 免费国产一区二区视频| 久久精品国产99久久久| 永久免费看啪啪网址入口| 久久久久久亚洲AV成人无码国产| av中文字幕在线资源网| 亚洲第一幕一区二区三区在线观看| 绝顶潮喷绝叫在线观看| 亚洲国产无线乱码在线观看 | 美妇炮灰被狂躁爽到高潮h| 夜夜揉揉日日人人青青| 亚洲av无码一区二区二三区下载| 一区二区三区婷婷中文字幕| 自拍成人免费在线视频| 国精品午夜福利视频不卡| 久久亚洲黄色| 亚洲av第一区综合激情久久久| 91色老久久偷偷精品蜜臀懂色| 在线涩涩免费观看国产精品| 亚洲视频1区| 中文字幕一区二区三区精品在线 | 日本国产精品高清在线| 久久影院午夜理论片无码| 日日碰狠狠添天天爽超碰97| 国产亚洲欧美另类第一页| 日本少妇熟女一区二区| 蜜桃一区二区三区| 中文字幕亚洲无线码在一区| 中文少妇一区二区三区| 国产av激情舒服刺激| 国精品无码一区二区三区在线| 久久中文字幕日韩无码视频| 久久成人永久婷婷99精品| 精品久久久久久久久午夜福利| 无码欧亚熟妇人妻AV在线外遇| 中文字幕av久久激情亚洲精品| 亚洲最大水蜜桃在线观看| 男人扒开女人下面狂躁小视频| 911国产在线观看精品| 中文字幕亚洲入口久久 | 亚洲一区二区综合精品|