亚洲免费av电影一区二区三区,日韩爱爱视频,51精品视频一区二区三区,91视频爱爱,日韩欧美在线播放视频,中文字幕少妇AV,亚洲电影中文字幕,久久久久亚洲av成人网址,久久综合视频网站,国产在线不卡免费播放

        ?

        Racism: Further Considerations from Psychological Science 心理科學對種族歧視的深度思考

        2022-01-18 19:58:09丁占罡
        英語世界 2022年1期

        丁占罡

        Racism is undeniably a matter of concern across countries and cultures. In the United States, where slavery was abolished in 1865 and segregation outlawed in 1954, the effects of racism are still pervasive in everyday life. Here is a look at what psychological scientists have uncovered on the topic in recent years.

        Systemic racism and implicit biases1

        Racism is usually defined by individual psychological processes such as prejudice, stereotyping, or discrimination. But racism is not only ingrained in individual minds; it is also found at historical and cultural levels. Focusing on individual prejudice can obscure the role that institutional and cultural processes play in maintaining race-based hierarchies. In a 2018 article in Current Directions in Psychological Science, Phia S. Salter (Davidson College), Glenn Adams (University of Kansas), and Michael J. Perez (Texas A&M University) proposed a cultural-psychological approach to racism. They suggested that racism is reproduced in everyday environments that afford2, promote, and maintain racist processes. People shape and maintain these racist processes through their preferences and actions. For example, people might select some representations of the past, shaping history and intervening in collective memory in a way that serves racism. This interplay between individuals and culture makes it more difficult to dismantle racism. According to Salter and colleagues3, racism cannot be dismantled by solely changing peoples individual biases because aspects of our everyday worlds support those biases.

        In Psychological Science, Heidi A. Vuletich and APS4 Fellow Keith Payne (University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill) analyzed individual biases and found that rather than being a property of individuals, implicit bias might be a property of social contexts, and that changing the social context (e.g., by increasing faculty diversity or removing Confederate monuments5 from campuses) may more effectively reduce bias than changing individual attitudes. In Vuletich and Paynes reanalysis of a previous study of 18 university campuses before and after a racial bias intervention, the researchers found that individual attitudes varied randomly across time and that campus characteristics such as low faculty diversity predicted high bias. This indicates that the stability of bias may reflect stable environments rather than persistent individual biases. Moreover, campuses characteristics that reflect historical and current inequalities, such as low faculty diversity, low social mobility (percentage of students whose parents had moved up from the poorest income quintile6), and display of Confederate monuments predicted high bias.

        Consequences of racism

        Racisms consequences are both physical and psychological. In Perspectives on Psychological Science, Antoinette M. Landor (University of Missouri) and Shardé McNeil Smith (University of Illinois) proposed that assaults resulting from an individuals skin tone7 may result in traumatic stress reactions and health and interpersonal outcomes (e.g., low self-esteem, hypertension, risky sexual behavior). Moreover, African Americans score worse than individuals with other racial backgrounds on most major physical health indicators, such as coronary heart disease, stroke, cancer, and HIV.

        Racism also results in economic and legal inequalities. For example, Rebecca C. Hetey and APS Fellow Jennifer L. Eberhardt (Stanford University) reported that there is plenty of evidence for racial disparities in the criminal justice system, where Blacks are much more likely to be punished than Whites. Michael W. Kraus, Ivuoma N. Onyeador, Natalie M. Daumeyer, Julian M. Rucker, and APS Fellow Jennifer A. Richeson (Yale University) suggested in Perspectives on Psychological Science that Americans vastly underestimate current racial economic inequality, especially the racial wealth gap. When the researchers compared participants estimates of economic disparities between White and Black families with actual economic data collected by the U.S. government, they found that participants underestimated the racial wealth gap in 1963 by 40% and the racial wealth gap in 2016 by 80%. Kraus and colleagues argued that these underestimates of inequality are driven, in part, by the salience8 of exemplars9 that bolster the belief that oppression has been overcome (e.g., Oprah Winfrey) and by motivations to perceive society as fair and economic status dependent only on merit.

        Ways to combat racism

        Reminding the public of dispar-ities between the treatment of Blacks and Whites might not be the best approach to combat racism. For example, in Current Directions in Psychological Science, Rebecca C. Hetey and APS Fellow Jennifer L. Eberhardt suggested that reminders about how Blacks are more likely to be punished by the crim-inal justice system than Whites may trigger fear and increase stereotypical associations between Blacks and crime, increasing biases and support for the policies that created the disparities. Instead, they suggest presenting data on racial disparities in a way that emphasizes that they are not natural, but rather the result of institutions that perpetuate10 structural and sociocultural forms of racism.

        Multiple researchers have investigated how contact with diversity should be managed to effectively combat ra-cism. Maureen A. Craig (New York University), Rucker, and Richeson proposed a framework to explain emerging data on how increases in racial diversity affect relationships among racial groups. In this framework, anticipated increases in diversity increase the perceived size of minority groups. Perceiving the minority group as larger can lead to an intensification of perceived threat to the dominant groups position in society and culture. Increases in perceived threat, in turn, impact the relationships among the racial groups, leading to augmented bias, prejudice, discrimination, and support for anti-immigrant policies. But these negative outcomes can be attenuated11 if increases in diversity are accompanied by positive individual experiences across racial groups. Positive contact with members of different racial groups, especially when individuals share equal status and common goals, can reduce perceptions of threat and, consequently, biased attitudes and behaviors. Increased diversity can increase opportunities for positive contact, but individual factors such as racial identification or beliefs about? stereotypes will also influence whether and the extent to which positive contact can reduce perceived threat.

        Regarding perceptions of economic equality, Kraus and colleagues suggested that, besides actually increasing economic equality, providing information about the actual state of racial economic disparities and evidence regarding the progress that has been made (or not) may help to reduce peoples misperceptions of racial equality. Kraus and colleagues added that understanding the pattern of racial economic inequality may help to understand racism in the context of the societal structures that maintain racial advantages and disadvantages.

        Recently, Mitchell R. Campbell and Markus Brauer (University of Wisconsin-Madison) suggested in Perspectives on Psychological Science that using principles from social marketing in prejudice research might help to minimize the existing gap between what we know about prejudice and the real-world methods to reduce it, which often fail. Social marketing is mainly concerned with changing behaviors; rather than identifying general principles in human behavior, it is problem-based and addresses specific behaviors in specific contexts. Campbell and Brauer advocated for the combined use of theory-based and problem-based principles to provide additional tools to help practitioners reduce prejudice and advance theory.

        毋庸置疑,種族歧視是各個國家、各種文化都關(guān)注的問題。美國1865年廢除了奴隸制,1954年取締種族隔離,但種族歧視的影響仍見于日常生活的方方面面。本文介紹心理學家近年來在這一問題上的發(fā)現(xiàn)。

        系統(tǒng)性種族歧視與內(nèi)隱偏見

        按照常規(guī)定義,種族歧視表現(xiàn)為一種個體的心理過程,比如成見、刻板印象或區(qū)別對待等。然而,種族歧視不僅植根于個體頭腦中,它還存在于歷史和文化層面。僅關(guān)注個體成見會掩蓋制度和文化過程在維護基于種族的等級制度上所起的作用。2018年,菲婭·S. 索爾特(戴維森學院)、格倫·亞當斯(堪薩斯大學)和邁克爾·J. 佩雷斯(得克薩斯農(nóng)機大學)在《最新心理科學指南》雜志上發(fā)表論文,從文化心理學視角探討種族歧視問題。他們認為,種族歧視在造就、推動、維持種族歧視過程的日常環(huán)境中繁衍。人們通過各自的偏好與行動塑造并維持這些歧視過程。比如,人們可能會選取一些過往的代表性事件,以有利于種族歧視的方式塑造歷史、干預集體記憶。這種個體與文化間的相互作用使得消除種族歧視變得更為困難。按照索爾特及兩位共同作者的觀點,僅靠改變?nèi)藗兊膫€體偏見無法消除種族歧視,因為我們?nèi)粘I畹母鱾€層面都在助長著那些偏見。

        海迪·A. 武萊蒂奇和心理學會會士基思·佩恩(北卡羅來納大學教堂山分校)在《心理學》雜志撰文分析了個體偏見,他們發(fā)現(xiàn)內(nèi)隱偏見并非個體屬性,而可能是一種社會環(huán)境屬性,且與改變個體態(tài)度相比,改變社會環(huán)境(比如,提升教職員工多樣性或從校園中移除南方邦聯(lián)紀念碑)可以更為有效地減輕偏見。此前有一項關(guān)于種族偏見干預前后的18所大學校園的研究,武萊蒂奇和佩恩對其進行重新分析后發(fā)現(xiàn),個體態(tài)度會因時間而異,且教職員工多樣性低這樣的校園特征預示著存在嚴重偏見。這表明,偏見的穩(wěn)定性可能反映的是穩(wěn)定的環(huán)境,而不是持續(xù)存在的個體偏見。此外,反映歷史和現(xiàn)時不平等的校園特征,比如教職員工多樣性低、社會流動性(父母收入從最低五分位向上提升的學生比例)低、建有南方邦聯(lián)紀念碑等,預示著存在嚴重偏見。

        種族歧視的影響

        種族歧視的影響既表現(xiàn)在身體層面,也表現(xiàn)在心理層面。安托瓦妮特·M. 蘭多爾(密蘇里大學)和沙爾德·麥克尼爾·史密斯(伊利諾伊大學)在《心理學展望》雜志上撰文指出,一個人因膚色招致的攻擊可導致創(chuàng)傷性應激反應以及健康和人際關(guān)系后果(比如自卑、過度緊張、危險的性行為等)。此外,與其他種族背景的人相比,非洲裔美國人在冠心病、中風、癌癥、艾滋病等多數(shù)主要身體健康指標上得分更低。

        種族歧視也會造成經(jīng)濟及法律上的不平等。例如,麗貝卡·C. 海泰伊和心理學會會士珍妮弗·L. 埃伯哈特(斯坦福大學)認為,有大量證據(jù)表明刑事司法系統(tǒng)內(nèi)存在種族差異,黑人比白人更有可能遭受懲罰。邁克爾·W. 克勞斯、伊沃瑪·N. 奧尼亞多爾、納塔莉·M. 多梅耶爾、朱利安·M. 拉克以及心理學會會士珍妮弗·A. 里奇森(耶魯大學)在《心理學展望》雜志上撰文指出,美國人大大低估了現(xiàn)時種族間的經(jīng)濟不平等,尤其是種族間的財富差距。研究人員曾讓受訪對象估算白人家庭和黑人家庭之間的經(jīng)濟差異,并將估算值與美國政府收集的實際經(jīng)濟數(shù)據(jù)進行對比,結(jié)果發(fā)現(xiàn),受訪對象對1963年種族間財富差距低估了40%,對2016年種族間財富差距低估高達80%??藙谒辜皫孜还餐髡咧赋?,受訪對象低估了不平等,這在一定程度上是因為讓其堅信壓迫已不復存在的事例(如奧普拉·溫弗瑞)凸現(xiàn)于世,同時也是因為他們受到激勵,認為社會是公平的,經(jīng)濟地位僅僅取決于個人才能。

        怎樣減少種族歧視

        提醒人們黑人與白人之間存在待遇差異,也許并不是減少種族歧視的最佳方式。例如,麗貝卡·C. 海泰伊和心理學會會士珍妮弗·L. 埃伯哈特在《最新心理科學指南》雜志上撰文指出,黑人如何比白人更可能遭受刑事司法制度的懲罰這樣的提示信息會引發(fā)恐懼,使人們更多地出于刻板印象在黑人與犯罪之間產(chǎn)生聯(lián)想,從而加劇偏見,為造成這種差異的政策提供更多助力。他們建議以如下方式展示種族差異的相關(guān)數(shù)據(jù):強調(diào)種族差異并非與生俱來,而是使種族歧視的結(jié)構(gòu)形式與社會文化形式得以延續(xù)的制度帶來的。

        多位研究人員調(diào)查了如何掌握人際接觸多樣性以便富有成效地減少種族歧視。莫琳·A. 克雷格(紐約大學)、拉克和里奇森提出了一個框架,用來解釋有關(guān)增強種族多樣性影響不同族群間關(guān)系的新數(shù)據(jù)。在該框架中,預期多樣性增強會擴大人們感覺中的少數(shù)族群規(guī)模,而人們感覺到的少數(shù)族群規(guī)模變大會使主導族群在社會、文化地位方面所感受到的威脅加劇。感受到的威脅增強進而又會影響不同族群間的關(guān)系,致使偏見、成見、區(qū)別對待激化,反移民政策得到進一步支持。然而,如果在增強多樣性的同時加入不同族群間積極的個體經(jīng)驗,這些消極后果就可以減少。與不同族群的成員積極接觸,尤其是在不同個體擁有平等地位和共同目標的情況下進行接觸,能夠減輕感受到的威脅,由此,帶有偏見的態(tài)度和行為也會減少。增強多樣性能夠增加積極接觸的機會,但是種族認同和有關(guān)刻板印象的看法等個體因素,也會對積極接觸能否以及在多大程度上減輕感受到的威脅產(chǎn)生影響。

        關(guān)于經(jīng)濟平等,克勞斯及幾位共同作者認為,除了實際上增進經(jīng)濟平等以外,提供有關(guān)種族經(jīng)濟差異實際狀況的信息和已經(jīng)取得(或未取得)的相關(guān)進展的證據(jù),或可有助于減少人們對種族平等的誤解??藙谒辜皫孜还餐髡哌€指出,理解種族經(jīng)濟不平等的模式,也許有助于人們結(jié)合使種族優(yōu)勢和劣勢得以維持的社會結(jié)構(gòu)背景來理解種族歧視。

        前不久,米切爾·R. 坎貝爾和馬庫斯·布勞爾(威斯康星大學麥迪遜分校)在《心理學展望》雜志上撰文指出,我們對成見的了解與現(xiàn)實世界為減少成見而采取的辦法(這些辦法往往無果而終)之間存在缺口。把社會營銷原理用于成見研究,可有助于縮小這一缺口。社會營銷主要涉及改變行為,它并非要明確人類行為的一般原理,而是以問題為基礎(chǔ),針對特定背景下的特定行為??藏悹柡筒紕跔柍珜⒒诶碚摰脑砗突趩栴}的原理合而用之,向?qū)I(yè)工作者提供更多手段,幫助他們減少成見、提出理論。

        (譯者為“《英語世界》杯”翻譯大賽獲獎者;單位:中華女子學院)

        日韩精品在线观看在线| 国产亚洲午夜精品| 日韩精品极品免费观看| 在教室轮流澡到高潮h免费视| 狠狠噜狠狠狠狠丁香五月| 欧美日韩中文国产一区| 亚洲欧美日韩在线精品2021| 美女视频黄a视频全免费网站色| 免费国产黄网站在线观看视频| 不卡高清av手机在线观看| 亚洲国产精品久久九色| 国产一区二区三区护士| 久久精品国产亚洲av果冻传媒| 国产成人麻豆精品午夜福利在线| 99综合精品久久| 国产免费人成视频在线观看| 又色又爽又黄高潮的免费视频| 亚洲的天堂av无码| 五月激情狠狠开心五月| 精品国产精品三级在线专区| 激情内射日本一区二区三区| 国产一级片毛片| 亚洲国产线茬精品成av| 手机在线看片| 无码少妇一区二区三区| 综合图区亚洲另类偷窥| 99久久国产精品免费热| 精品少妇一区二区三区免费观| 99热这里只有精品69| 国产精品国产三级国a| 激情内射亚洲一区二区三区| 国产精品永久免费视频| 激,情四虎欧美视频图片| 在线观看国产视频你懂得| 成人网站免费看黄a站视频 | 在线观看亚洲AV日韩A∨| 国产女主播一区二区三区在线观看| 超碰人人超碰人人| .精品久久久麻豆国产精品| 魔鬼身材极品女神在线| 99噜噜噜在线播放|