亚洲免费av电影一区二区三区,日韩爱爱视频,51精品视频一区二区三区,91视频爱爱,日韩欧美在线播放视频,中文字幕少妇AV,亚洲电影中文字幕,久久久久亚洲av成人网址,久久综合视频网站,国产在线不卡免费播放

        ?

        Case-control study of error-related negativity among males with heroin dependence undergoing rehabilitation

        2013-12-09 03:28:35HongCHENHaifengJIANGQianGUOJiangDUJijunWANGMinZHAO
        上海精神醫(yī)學(xué) 2013年3期
        關(guān)鍵詞:差異

        Hong CHEN, Haifeng JIANG, Qian GUO, Jiang DU, Jijun WANG*, Min ZHAO*

        ·Original Article·

        Case-control study of error-related negativity among males with heroin dependence undergoing rehabilitation

        Hong CHEN, Haifeng JIANG, Qian GUO, Jiang DU, Jijun WANG*, Min ZHAO*

        1. Introduction

        By the end of 2011 there were 1.2 million registered heroin users in China.[1]Heroin dependence has, thus,become a serious public health problem for the country.But research on the causes, characteristics and course of heroin addiction in China is quite limited, so there are no culture-specific theoretical models that can help inform preventive and therapeutic interventions.

        One area of interest is the cognitive functioning of persons who become addicted to heroin. Previous studies have found impairments of several dimensions of cognitive functioning among individuals with heroin dependence, including impulse control, attention, and the processing of emotional or cognitive stimuli.[2-4]But few studies have assessed high-level cognitive functions.One measure of high-level cognitive functioning is error-related negativity (ERN), an indicator of the selfmonitoring mechanisms of the human brain after the occurrence of an error.[5]ERN is measured by assessing the sharp negative electroencephalographic (EEG) signal when an incorrect motor response is made during behavioral tests of event-related potentials (ERP). This ERN wave typically occurs concurrently with or 50-100 ms after the erroneous response, has an amplitude of about 10 μv, and is most prominent in the frontal and central EEG electrodes.[5,6]Most studies of ERN have been conducted in persons without mental disorders,[7,8]though some studies have been reported in persons with schizophrenia[9]and anxiety disorders.[10]We have found no reports of ERP-based studies about self-monitoring mechanisms among individuals with heroin dependence either in China or in other countries. With the goal of advancing understanding of the neurobiologicalmechanisms of heroin dependence, this study uses highdensity ERP techniques to compare the characteristics of ERN among males undergoing rehabilitation for heroin dependence and non-dependent, male community members.

        2. Methods

        2.1 Sample

        The enrolment of subjects is shown in Figure 1. The heroin-dependent group consisted of 20 men who entered the heroin dependence rehabilitation program at the First Compulsory Rehabilitation Center of Shanghai from September 2009 to August 2010. Inclusion criteria were: (a) 18-55 years of age, (b) meets diagnostic criteria of substance dependence listed in the fourth edition of the American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV),[11](c)no history of use of other drugs, (d) no history of mental retardation, serious physical illness, or serious mental illness, and (e) right-handedness. The mean (sd) age of the 20 individuals was 37.1 (9.5) years, the mean duration of formal education was 8.4 (3.0) years, the mean duration of prior heroin use was 12.4 (3.1) years,and the mean time of residential treatment in the current rehabilitation program was 8.2 (2.0) months (range, 4-10 months). Four were in their first episode of the mandated two-compulsory inpatient treatment (which lasts two years), 16 had had prior periods of compulsory treatment.

        The control group consisted of 15 individuals recruited from male staff members in the Shanghai Mental Health Center. Inclusion criteria included (a) 18-55 years of age,(b) no current or prior mental illness or mental retardation identified during a clinical interview, (c) no history of drug abuse, (d) no serious physical illness, and (e) righthandedness. The mean age of the control group was 32.5 (9.9) years and they had a mean of 13.7 (4.6) years of education. There was no significant difference in the mean age between cases and controls but the duration of formal schooling in the controls was significantly longer than in the cases (Z=4.08, df=33, p=0.001).

        The current study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Shanghai Mental Health Center. All participants were informed about the procedures of the study and signed consent forms.

        Figure 1. Flowchart of the study

        2.2 Assessment

        All participants filled out a general information questionnaire, which collected demographic information including age, level of education and marital status. In the patient group it also collected information related to drug use including age at the time of the first use of illegal drugs, duration of usage, frequency of use, route(s) of administration, number of attempts at quitting, and time in current compulsory inpatient treatment program.

        ERN induced by Eriksen flanker tests[12]were recorded using EEG. All tests were conducted using standardized instructions and parameters by a single trained technician using equipment provided by the Brain Products (Germany) Company. Tests were conducted in a private room that was quiet and had low lighting. During the test, participants wore an electroencephalography cap and sat in front of a monitor at a distance of 100 cm. The ERP were recorded using the international standard 32-channel Ag-Agcl EEG cap. The fronto-central electrode was used as the reference and the anterior midline frontal (AFz) electrode was used as the ground.All electrodes were connected to the scalp via conductive gel. (In order to reduce interference while wearing the EEG cap, all participants washed their hair prior to the test.) Resistance was lower than 5 kΩ, sampling rate was 1000 Hz, and the range of the recording band width was 0.01-200 Hz.

        During each trial, a “+” symbol was initially shown on the screen for a random duration that varied in length between 400 and 700 ms. This was followed by a 125ms presentation of one of four stimuli (a string of five letters in the form of ‘HHHHH’, ‘AAAAA’, ‘HHAHH’, or‘AAHAA’) that were presented as images 1.5 cm x 6 cm on the monitor. At the beginning of the test, participants were instructed to react to these stimuli as follows:“Use the left hand to press ‘1’ (on the keyboard) when the letter in the middle is ‘H’ and use the right hand to press ‘5’ when the letter in the middle is ‘A’. Please press the button as quickly as possible.” Each presentation of a stimulus was followed by a black screen for 475 ms,during which time the participant was expected to react to the stimulus as requested. This is followed by a black screen for a further 500 ms before beginning the next trial; if the participant did not react to the stimulus, the words ‘hurry up’ were shown on the screen for 500 ms before the next stimulus was presented. A brief pilot test was conducted first to familiarize the participant with the process and requirements of the test. The main test included 800 trails in eight blocks with 100 trails in each block and a 30-second rest between blocks. Each of the four stimuli was presented 200 times during the entire test, which took 25 minutes to complete.

        2.3 Data analysis

        The Analyzer 1.02 software provided by the Brain Products Company was used to conduct offline analyses of the ERN data. All data were referenced to the average of the reference electrodes and then band-pass filtered between 0.5 and 30 Hz. The time point when the participant pressed the button served as the baselinetime of the response. EEG data were segmented for each stimulus beginning 150ms before the onset of the response and continuing until 550 ms after the response.Ocular corrections, and baseline corrections were made automatically, and artifacts (>100 μV) were automatically deleted. Data for responses that occurred after erroneous responses (minimum of 20 erroneous responses) were then pooled to quantify the ERN measures for each of the three frontal midline EEG electrodes (Fz,FCz and Cz). Tests during which the respondent made less than 21 errors were excluded from the analysis. The ERN was the EEG wave that occurred during the time window of 0-100 ms after the incorrect motor response. The amplitude of the crude ERN wave (i.e., the maximum value of the mean negative wave following incorrect responses) and latency of these waves (i.e., the time at which the mean raw ERN waves reached their maximum values) were measured. To increase the sensitivity of the analysis, we also computed the amplitude and latency of a ‘standardized’ ERN wave, a composite wave, which subtracts the mean wave after correct responses from the mean wave after incorrect responses. These four values were used in the current analysis.

        SPSS 13.0 statistical software was used for dataanalysis. Non-parametric tests and t-tests were used to analyze demographic data and behavioral data;repeated measures analysis of variance was used to analyze the EEG data. Because EEG data did not follow normal distributions, Spearman correlation coefficients were used to estimate the correlation between EEG measures and other variables. Level of education was categorized into six groups (i.e., 0 years, 1-6 years, 7-9 years, 10-12 years, 13-15 years, and 16+ years) and was entered in the analyses as a covariate. The level of statistical significance was set at p<0.05.

        Table 1. Comparison of behavioral results for the Erickson flanker tests between heroindependent patients and controls

        Figure 2. ERN waves of the case and control groups

        Figure 3. Standardized ERN waves of the case and control groups

        3. Results

        3.1 Comparison of behavioral data for the Eriksen flanker tests

        Comparison of test results of patients and controls is shown in Table 1. There were no statistically significant differences between the two groups in non-response rates, error rates, or accuracy. There was, however, a wider variation in the number of errors in the heroin group (ranging from 18 to 373 errors in the 800 trials)than in the control group (38 to 107 errors). Compared to the control group, heroin-dependent patients hadshorter response times both for correct and erroneous responses. After including levels of education as a covariate, these differences in response times remained statistically significant (F=51.55, p<0.001 for correct responses; and F=40.97, p<0.001 for incorrect responses). The reaction time for erroneous responses was shorter than that for correct responses for both patients (paired t=5.75, p<0.001) and for controls (paired t=8.85, p<0.001).

        Table 2. Comparisons of mean (sd) amplitudes and latencies of crude and standardized ERN waves between patients and controls

        3.2 Comparison of amplitudes and latencies of the crude and standardized ERN waves

        After eliminating three participants from the patient group who had less than 20 erroneous responses, ERN data from the EEG results of three frontal electrodes (Fz,FCz and Cz) were compared between 17 patients and 15 controls.

        The crude ERN waves at the three electrodes for the patient and control groups are shown in Figure 2 and the univariate comparison of the amplitudes and latencies of these waves is shown in Table 2. Repeated measures ANOVA found no significant difference between patients and controls in the amplitude of the crude ERN wave(F=1.32, p=0.260) but there was a statistically significant difference in amplitude between the three electrodes(F=12.86, p<0.001). The interaction between group and electrode location was not statistically significant(F=3.07, p=0.064). The significant difference in crude ERN amplitude by electrode site disappeared after adjustment for the level of education (F=0.80, p=0.435).The latency of the crude ERN waves was not significantly different at the three electrode sites (F=1.01, p=0.341),but it was significantly shorter in the patient group than in the control group (F=17.63, p<0.001), and this difference in latency remained significant after adjustment for education (F=15.12, p<0.001).

        The standardized ERN waves for the patient and control groups are shown in Figure 3 and the amplitudes and latencies of these waves are shown in Table 2.Repeated measures ANOVA found statistically significant differences in the amplitude of the standardized ERN wave between the two groups (F=4.46, p=0.021)and between the three electrode locations (F=13.81,p<0.001). No significant interaction effects between group and electrode locations were found (F=1.01,p=0.341). After adjustment for educational differences between the groups, the differences in amplitude by group (F=3.53, p=0.070) and by electrode location(F=1.79, p=0.191) were no longer statistically significant.The latency of the standardized ERN waves was significantly shorter in patients than in controls, and this difference remained significant after adjustment for educational level (F=4.69, p=0.039). However, the latency of standardized ERN waves was not significantly different at the three electrode placements (F=0.14,p=0.871), and the interaction term between group and electrode location was not significant (F=1.27, p=0.288).

        3.3 Correlations between ERN and variables related to heroin use in the heroin-dependence group

        Duration of heroin use was significantly correlated with the amplitude of the standardized ERN wave at the FCz area (Spearman correlation coefficient, rs=0.52,p=0.032) and at the Cz area (rs=0.53, p=0.029) but not at the Fz area. However, duration of heroin use was not significantly correlated with the latency of the standardized ERN wave at any of the three electrodes.The standardized ERN amplitude at the FCz area among patients was also significantly correlated with age(rs=0.62, p=0.008); a correlation that was not observed in the control group. No statistically significant correlations were found between the amplitude or latency of the standardized ERN wave and the level of education, age at first heroin use, or number of relapses.

        4. Discussion

        4.1 Main findings

        This study is the first ERP study on error-monitoring in individuals with heroin dependence. We applied the widely used Erikson flanker tasks to provoke ERN waves on the EEG when erroneous responses are induced by presenting incongruent flankers around the target stimulus. We found no significant differences in the error rates for patients with a history of heroin addiction compared to controls who had no history of substance abuse or other mental disorder, but the response times for both correct and incorrect responses were shorter in the heroin-dependent patients than in the controls.The amplitude of the crude and standardized ERN waves was lower in patients than controls, but this difference became non-significant after adjustment for the lower educational level in the patient group. Consistent with the reaction time results, the latency of both crude and standardized ERN waves was significantly shorter in the patient group and this difference remained significant after adjustment for educational level. The more rapid reaction times and shorter latencies in our patients suggest that persons with a history of substance abuse are more impulsive than those without such a history,a finding that is in line with studies that have reported neurophysiological indicators of impaired impulse control in heroin-dependent patients.[1]

        Overall, our results suggest - but do not prove -abnormalities in error-monitoring among individuals who have a history of heroin dependence. These results are largely consistent with other studies of errormonitoring among individuals with addictive disorders and other types of mental disorders. Previous studies reported decreased ERN amplitude in individuals with alcohol dependence,[13]cocaine dependence,[14]cannabis dependence,[15]and internet addiction.[16]Other studies have also reported decreased ERN amplitude in individuals with schizophrenia.[9]In contrast, studies of individuals with anxiety disorders report higher ERN amplitude.[10]

        In the patient group we also found a significant correlation between the negative amplitude of the standardized ERN wave and the duration of heroin use, suggesting that the damage to error-monitoring functions may be progressive. Previous studies about the relationship of duration of abuse and severity of cognitive impairments have been inconsistent: Hill and colleagues[17]found a positive association between the degree of cognitive dysfunction and the duration of heroin abuse, while Guerra and colleagues[18]found no such relationship.

        4.2 Limitations

        This study had a relatively small sample size and was limited to male subjects who had been in a compulsory rehabilitation facility for several months. Thus the sample is not representative of all heroin-dependent individuals in China and some of the non-significant results may be due to Type II errors. The failure to match patients and controls on educational status had a substantial effect on the results; after adjustment for educational level several of the statistically significant differences disappeared. Future studies in this are clearly need to match cases and controls by education. The crosssectional nature of the study made it impossible to determine whether the changes in ERN identified in the patient group are state phenomena, trait phenomena,or have both state and trait components. It is essential to conduct longitudinal studies to distinguish these different potential etiologies for ERN abnormalities.Such studies would help determine how best to employ these measures: if the ERN differences in individuals with a history of heroin addiction are traits, then they can be used to identify persons at high risk of developing addictive conditions; if, on the other hand, the ERN abnormalities are state variables, it may be possible to use them as biological markers of the severity of disease or of the effectiveness of interventions.

        4.3 Significance

        This study is the first to report on the characteristics of ERN among patients with a history of heroin dependence. Compared to control subjects, heroindependent patients had clear evidence of shortened reaction times and ERN latency. Consistent with ERN studies in other addictive conditions, we also found a non-significant reduction in ERN amplitude and a significant correlation between duration of heroin use and the negativity of the standardized ERN wave. These findings suggest abnormal error-monitoring functions in persons with heroin dependence and add to the growing literature on the neurological mechanisms related to cognitive dysfunction in individuals with addictive disorders. Further research with larger samples that integrate the neuropsycholgical tests with neuroimaging analyses are needed to move this important line of inquiry forward.

        Conflict of interest

        The authors declare no conflict of interest.

        Acknowledgement

        Authors are grateful for the support and help from staff at the Shanghai First Compulsory Rehabilitation Center during this project.

        Funding

        The current study was funded by the National Science Foundation of China (81271468, 81130020, 81171267),the Pillar Projects of the Twelfth 5-year Plan of China(2012BAI01B07), the New 100-People Plan of the health system in the city of Shanghai, the National Key Specialized Clinical Medicine-Shanghai Mental Health Center (Medical Administrative Department of the Ministry of Health, 2011-873), and the Shanghai Health Bureau Foundation (2011Y097).

        1. Committee on Illicit Drug Use. 2012 Report on Illicit Drugs.Beijing: Committee on Illicit Drug Use, 2012. (in Chinese)

        2. Lin B, Qian RB, Fu XM, Hu WF, Yin T, Niu CS, et al. Impulsive behavior in heroin addicts: a P300 ERP study. Chinese Journal of Behavioral Medicine and Brain Science 2012; 21(3): 235-237. (in Chinese)

        3. Li W, Chen DY, Hu CF, Hao W, Wu DX, Yao SQ. A study on attention and auditory P300 in male patients with heroin dependence in a stage of rehabilitation. Journal of Psychiatry 2008; 21(2): 81-83. (in Chinese)

        4. Jiang YP, Xu P, Wang Y, Lu GH. Effects of electroacupuncture on event-related potentials in male heroin addicts’ emotion attention. Shanghai Journal of Traditional Chinese Medicine 2007; 41(5): 60-62. (in Chinese)

        5. Yang WM, Xiao ZP, Zhang M, Li H, Tang YY, Wang JJ. A comparative study of error-related negativity (ERN) between obsessive-compulsive disorder and of schizophrenia. Chinese Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases 2011; 37(6): 370-373. (in Chinese)

        6. Schefers MK, Coles MG, Bernstein P, Gehring WJ, Donchin E.Event-related brain potentials and error-related processing:an analysis of incorrect responses to go and no-go stimuli.Psychophysiology 1996; 33(1): 42-53.

        7. Miller AE, Watson JM, Strayer DL. Individual differences in working memory capacity predict action monitoring and the error-related negativity. J Exp Psychol Learn 2012; 38(3): 757-763.

        8. Themanson JR, Rosen PJ, Pontifex MB, Hillman CH, McAuley E. Alteration in error-related brain activity and post-error behavior over time. Brain Cognition 2012; 80(2): 257-265.

        9. Chen XS, Xu YF, Tang YX, Wang Y, Zhang MD, Lou FY, et al.Preliminary study on variations and neural generators of error-related negativity in first episode schizophrenics.National Medical Journal of China 2011; 91(43): 3040-3043.(in Chinese)

        10. Xiao Z, Wang J, Zhang M, Li H, Tang Y, Wang Y, et al. Error-related negativity abnormalities in generalized anxiety disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2011; 35(1): 265-272.

        11. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4thed. Washington: American Psychiatric Assoation; 1994.

        12. Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW. Effects of noise letters upon identification of a target letter in a non-search task. Percept Psychophys 1974; 16: 143-149.

        13. Easdona C, Izenberga A, Armilioa ML, Yu H , Alaina C. Alcohol consumption impairs stimulus- and error-related processing during a Go/No-Go Task. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2005;25(3): 873-883.

        14. Franken IH, van Strien JW, Franzek EJ, van de Wetering BJ. Error-processing deficits in patients with cocaine dependence. Biol Psychol 2007; 75(1): 45-51.

        15. Hester R, Nestor L, Garavan H. Impaired error awareness and anterior cingulated cortex hypoactivity in chronic cannabis users. Neuropsychopharmacology 2009; 34(11): 2450-2458.

        16. Wang Y, Zhai TY, Sheng WB, Shao YC, Zhang Y, Ma HC, et al.Electrophysiological evidence for abnormal error monitoring in people with internet addiction disorder. Chinese Journal of Drug Dependence 2012; 21(3): 197-199. (in Chinese)

        17. Hill SY, Mikhael MA. Computerized transaxial tomographic and neuropsychol evalutions in chronic alcoholics and heroin abusers. Am J Psychiatry 1979; 136(4-B): 598-602.

        18. Guerra D, Sole A, Cami J, Tobena A. Neuropsychological performance in opiate addicts after rapid detoxification. Drug Alcohol Depend, 1987; 20(3): 261-270.

        康復(fù)期男性海洛因依賴者的錯(cuò)誤相關(guān)負(fù)電位的病例對(duì)照研究

        陳紅 江海峰 郭茜 杜江 王繼軍* 趙敏*

        上海交通大學(xué)醫(yī)學(xué)院附屬精神衛(wèi)生中心 上海

        背景: 截止2011年底,全國(guó)登記吸毒人群中濫用海洛因人員共115.6萬(wàn)人,然而國(guó)內(nèi)對(duì)海洛因依賴者神經(jīng)心理功能的研究較少。目的探討康復(fù)期男性海洛因依賴者高級(jí)認(rèn)知功能的重要指標(biāo)之一,即錯(cuò)誤相關(guān)負(fù)電位(error-related negativity,ERN)的特征。方法符合DSM-IV診斷標(biāo)準(zhǔn)的男性海洛因依賴者20例和健康對(duì)照者15人完成視覺(jué)Eriksen Flanker刺激任務(wù)。該任務(wù)共800個(gè)刺激,在目標(biāo)刺激兩側(cè)呈現(xiàn)與目標(biāo)不完全一致的干擾刺激,以此誘發(fā)錯(cuò)誤應(yīng)答及ERN。記錄被試頭皮32導(dǎo)腦電圖(EEG)信號(hào)。比較患者組與對(duì)照組的平均錯(cuò)誤率和執(zhí)行任務(wù)的反應(yīng)時(shí),并比較額葉中線3處EEG電極位置(Fz、FCz和Cz)原始ERN以及ERN差異波(在錯(cuò)誤應(yīng)答波基礎(chǔ)上減去正確應(yīng)答波后的負(fù)波)的波幅和潛伏期。結(jié)果兩組的錯(cuò)誤率無(wú)明顯差異,但與對(duì)照組相比,患者組的正確反應(yīng)時(shí)和錯(cuò)誤反應(yīng)時(shí)均縮短。無(wú)論是原始ERN還是ERN差異波,患者組的波幅均低于對(duì)照組。由于患者組受教育年限較低,經(jīng)校正后,兩組波幅差異無(wú)統(tǒng)計(jì)學(xué)意義。患者組的原始ERN和ERN差異波的潛伏期均比對(duì)照組短,校正受教育水平后差異仍有統(tǒng)計(jì)學(xué)意義。海洛因依賴組ERN差異波波幅與吸毒年限呈正相關(guān)。結(jié)論研究結(jié)果提示有海洛因依賴史的個(gè)體存在沖動(dòng)控制能力受損以及錯(cuò)誤監(jiān)控功能異常。關(guān)于成癮障礙個(gè)體認(rèn)知功能障礙的神經(jīng)機(jī)制的研究日益增加,本研究也為這一文獻(xiàn)庫(kù)添磚加瓦。

        Background:There were 1.2 million registered heroin users in China by the end of 2011, but little research in the country has focused on the neuropsychological functioning of these individuals.Aim:Assess error-related negativity (ERN) - an important indicator of high-level cognitive functioning - among males with heroin dependence undergoing rehabilitation. Method: Twenty male patients in a rehabilitation center who met DSM-IV criteria for heroin dependence and 15 healthy male controls completed 800 trials of Erikson flanker tasks to provoke ERN waves on the 32-electrode electroencephalograph (EEG) when erroneous responses are induced by presenting incongruent flankers around the target stimulus. Mean error rates and reaction times for the task, and the amplitude and latency of crude ERN waves and standardized ERN waves (created by subtracting the wave for correct responses from that for incorrect responses) at three frontal midline EEG electrodes (Fz, FCz, and Cz) were compared between patients and controls.Results:There was no significant difference in error rates between patients and controls but the reaction times for both erroneous and correct responses were shorter in patients than controls. The amplitude of both crude and standardized ERN waves was lower in patients than controls, but this difference became non-significant after adjustment for the lower educational level in the patient group. The latency of the peak value in both the crude and standardized ERN waves was significantly shorter in the patient group; this difference remained significant after adjustment for educational level. There was a significant correlation between the negative amplitude of the standardized ERN wave and the duration of heroin use.Conclusion:These findings suggest impaired impulse control and abnormal error-monitoring functions in persons with a history of heroin dependence and add to the growing literature on the neurological mechanisms related to cognitive dysfunction in individuals with addictive disorders.

        10.3969/j.issn.1002-0829.2013.03.003

        Shanghai Mental Health Center, Shanghai Jiao Tong University School of Medicine, Shanghai, China

        *correspondence: jijunwang27@gmail.com; drminzhao@gmail.com

        (received: 2012-12-10, accepted: 2013-03-11 )

        Dr. Hong Chen graduated from Jining Medical School in June 2009 with a major of Clinical Medicine.In September 2010, she was enrolled in the master's program in Psychiatry and Mental Health at the Shanghai Mental Health Center, Shanghai Jiao Tong University School of Medicine. Her research interest is in drug dependence.

        *通信作者:jijunwang27@gmail.com(王繼軍);drminzhao@gmail.com(趙敏)

        猜你喜歡
        差異
        “再見(jiàn)”和bye-bye等表達(dá)的意義差異
        JT/T 782的2020版與2010版的差異分析
        相似與差異
        關(guān)于中西方繪畫差異及對(duì)未來(lái)發(fā)展的思考
        收藏界(2019年3期)2019-10-10 03:16:40
        找句子差異
        DL/T 868—2014與NB/T 47014—2011主要差異比較與分析
        生物為什么會(huì)有差異?
        法觀念差異下的境外NGO立法效應(yīng)
        構(gòu)式“A+NP1+NP2”與“A+NP1+(都)是+NP2”的關(guān)聯(lián)和差異
        論言語(yǔ)行為的得體性與禮貌的差異
        国产一区二区三区不卡在线播放| 国产亚洲精品久久久ai换| 麻豆精品国产精华液好用吗| 色视频综合无码一区二区三区| 99久久精品日本一区二区免费| 乱人伦中文字幕成人网站在线| 欧美日韩国产专区| 美腿丝袜av在线播放| 日韩人妻一区二区中文字幕| 亚洲天堂av在线网站| 国产成人综合日韩精品无码| 欧美乱妇高清无乱码在线观看| 国产性一交一乱一伦一色一情| 欧美国产亚洲日韩在线二区| 91青青草久久| 午夜蜜桃视频在线观看| 亚洲av熟女少妇久久| 色视频线观看在线网站| 国产亚洲精品久久久久婷婷瑜伽| 国产精品麻豆最新AV| 丰满熟妇人妻无码区| 亚洲成生人免费av毛片| 青青草激情视频在线播放| 天天躁日日躁狠狠躁欧美老妇小说| 中文字幕乱码熟女人妻水蜜桃| 99久久综合狠狠综合久久| 国产亚洲精品自在久久77| 亚洲精品一区二区三区播放| 日本人妖一区二区三区 | 性欧美丰满熟妇xxxx性久久久| 国产精品一区二区久久蜜桃| 国产一区二区三区四色av| 香蕉免费一区二区三区| 国外精品视频在线观看免费| 日本久久久| 久久精品国产亚洲av热一区| 久久国产精品美女厕所尿尿av| 亚洲本色精品一区二区久久| 中文字幕人妻丝袜成熟乱| 无码人妻久久一区二区三区免费| 玩弄放荡人妻一区二区三区|